Education, Science, Technology, Innovation and Life
Open Access
Sign In

The Impact of Conflict Situations on Subjective Well-being and the Moderating Role of Thought Suppression

Download as PDF

DOI: 10.23977/jsoce.2025.070214 | Downloads: 9 | Views: 428

Author(s)

Jingnan Wang 1, Min Ou 1

Affiliation(s)

1 School of Psychology, Northwest Normal University, Lanzhou, 730070, Gansu, China

Corresponding Author

Jingnan Wang

ABSTRACT

This study investigates the influence of conflict situations on subjective well-being and the moderating role of thought suppression. A questionnaire survey was conducted among adults in Lanzhou City using the Relationship Conflict Scale, Subjective Well-Being Scale, and White Bear Suppression Inventory. A total of 220 questionnaires were collected, with one invalid response excluded based on completion time, yielding 219 valid questionnaires (effective rate: 99.55%). Data were analyzed using SPSS. Results revealed: (1) A significant correlation between conflict situations and subjective well-being (ps<0.001); (2) Conflict situations negatively predicted subjective well-being (β = -0.225, t = -4.412, p<0.001); (3) Thought suppression partially moderated the effect of conflict situations on subjective well-being (β = -0.869, t = -3.098, p<0.01). Higher levels of conflict situations were associated with lower subjective well-being, but thought suppression alleviated the negative impact of conflicts. The findings suggest that employing thought suppression strategies appropriately in unavoidable conflict situations can mitigate adverse effects, enhance subjective well-being, improve adaptive capacity, and foster mental health by balancing psychological conflicts.

KEYWORDS

Conflict situations; Subjective well-being; Thought inhibition

CITE THIS PAPER

Jingnan Wang, Min Ou, The Impact of Conflict Situations on Subjective Well-being and the Moderating Role of Thought Suppression. Journal of Sociology and Ethnology (2025) Vol. 7: 95-100. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.23977/jsoce.2025.070214.

REFERENCES

[1] Diener E, Suh E M, Lucas R E, et al. Subjective Well-Being: Three Decades of Progress[J]. Psychological Bulletin, 1999, 125(2): 276302.
[2] Diener E. (1984). Subject Well-being[M]. Psychological Bulletin,1984,95(3):542-575.
[3] Duan, J. H. (1996). Application and analysis of the General Well-Being Scale in Chinese college students. Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology, (1), 56-57.
[4] Ren, J., Jin, Z. C., & Yang, Q. J. (2010). A meta-analysis of factors influencing subjective well-being in older adults. Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology, 18(1), 119-121.
[5] Niu, K. N., Li, M., & Zhang, X. K. (2021). The relationship between adolescent friendship quality and subjective well-being: A meta-analysis. Psychological Development and Education, 37(3), 407-418.
[6] Hu, Y. Q., Wang, B. R., & Li, Z. H. (2015). A correlational study on mental health, subjective well-being, and personality of "post-90s" college students. University Education Science, (2), 121-124.
[7] Zhang, Y., & Li, L. (2021). The impact of sports participation on subjective well-being: Evidence from sociological empirical research. Journal of Shenyang Sport University, 40(2), 92-102, 117.
[8] Yue, T., Wang, H., Li, Q. G., Ren, X. X., & Zhang, X. Y. (2025). Value conflicts from a psychological perspective: Impacts and theoretical explanations. Advances in Psychological Science, 33(2), 351-361.
[9] Wu, C. H. (2024). Preference for autonomous decision-making in approach-approach and avoidance-avoidance conflict situations (Master's thesis, Zhejiang Normal University).
[10] Zhou, S. H. (2024). The influence of emotional feedback on conflict processing (Master's thesis, Sichuan Normal University).
[11] Li, J. S., Guo, Y. L., Li, X. J., Shi, W. S., Shi, J. P., Yu, Y. M., & Xu, Y. X. (2022). Backfiring effects of thought suppression: Immediate enhancement and delayed rebound. Psychological Science, 45(4), 820-825. doi:10. 16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.20220407
[12] Phelps, E. A. (2006). Emotion and cognition: Insights from studies of the human amygdala. Annual review of psychology, 57, 27-53.
[13] Jehn, K. A. (1995). A multimethod examination of the benefits and detriments of intragroup conflict. Administrative Science Quarterly, 40(2),256-282. Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin,95(3),542–575.
[14] Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S. R., & White, T. L. (1987). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 53(1), 5-13.
[15] Zhou Lihua, Chen Jian & Su Linyan. (2012). Study on the validity of the Chinese version of the White Bear Inhibition Questionnaire. Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology, 20(03), 343-345.
[16] Chen, L. H., Chen, J., & Su, L. Y. (2012). Reliability and validity of the Chinese version of the White Bear Suppression Inventory. Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology, 20(3), 343-345.
[17] Liu, J. (2024). The relationship between adolescent perfectionism and subjective well-being: The mediating role of rumination, the moderating role of psychological capital, and intervention studies (Master's thesis, Hebei University).
[18] Andrew, Ward, Sonja, Lyubomirsky, Lorie, & Sousa, et al. (2003). Can't quite commit: rumination and uncertainty. Personality & social psychology bulletin. 

All published work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.

Copyright © 2016 - 2031 Clausius Scientific Press Inc. All Rights Reserved.